Which Method Is Not an Effective Way of Soothing a Crying Baby?
PLoS 1. 2019; 14(four): e0214548.
Infant crying and the calming response: Parental versus mechanical soothing using swaddling, sound, and motion
Eline 50. Möller
1 Stichting Epilepsie Instellingen Nederland, Heemstede, The Netherlands
ii Research Establish of Child Development and Instruction, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Kingdom of the netherlands
3 Research Priority Area Yield, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Wieke de Vente
ii Research Establish of Kid Evolution and Pedagogy, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
3 Research Priority Surface area Yield, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Roos Rodenburg
1 Stichting Epilepsie Instellingen Nederland, Heemstede, Kingdom of the netherlands
2 Research Establish of Child Evolution and Education, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The netherlands
3 Research Priority Expanse Yield, University of Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Anneloes van Baar, Editor
Received 2018 Dec 27; Accustomed 2019 Mar 14.
- Supplementary Materials
-
GUID: 6744B778-2EE4-48B7-80B4-FA7AE21D92E9
GUID: 3098F998-6DB4-4C6E-8139-85127BD953E3
- Information Availability Statement
-
All relevant information are inside the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
Abstract
Background
Frequent infant crying is associated with parental exhaustion, low, or even babe hospitalization and shaken baby syndrome. Effective prompt soothing methods are defective for infants nether half dozen months. We examined whether swaddling, audio, and move evoked an firsthand calming response (CR) when parents soothed their infants and using a smart crib, and whether infant age afflicted the CR.
Methods
Infants' CR was assessed in a community sample of 69 infants (0–6 months) in a balanced experiment with two atmospheric condition (parent, smart crib) each composed of iii ii-minute phases (baseline, supine, soothing). During baseline 1, parent and infant were sitting together; in supine 1, fussiness was elicited by putting the infant suddenly supine, followed by parental soothing (shushing and jiggling of the swaddled infant). Baseline two, supine 2, and soothing by the crib followed. Fussiness was observed and infant eye rate (HR) and middle rate variability (HRV) were recorded. The CR was operationalized as decreased fussiness and Hr, and increased HRV during soothing compared to lying supine.
Results
Infant fussiness and Hr were lower in both soothing phases compared to the supine phases. Infant HRV tended to exist higher during parental soothing than during supine, just did not significantly differ between mechanical soothing and supine. Younger infants responded with a stronger CR (decreased fussiness and increased HRV) to parental soothing, but not to mechanical soothing. For HR, infants' CR was stronger in the crib than in the parent status, whereas for HRV, infants' CR was stronger in the parent condition. For fussiness, infants' CR tended to be stronger in the parent condition.
Determination
Parental and mechanical soothing using swaddling, audio, and move promptly induced a CR in infants. This has important clinical implications for soothing fussy and crying infants. Future studies should investigate the effects of parental versus mechanical soothing in the dwelling house setting.
Introduction
Crying is part of normal infant beliefs and plays an important role in the mutual regulation between infant and parent. Yet, up to 20% of infants cry excessively [1]. Wessel's definition of baby excessive crying is often used: crying for at least three hours per 24-hour interval for at to the lowest degree iii days per week, and at to the lowest degree for three weeks in a row [ii]. In clinical practice, this definition is non very useful as parents may also perceive less frequent crying as a trouble. Crying is therefore considered excessive when parents experience it equally such [3].
Crying issues are burdening to infants and parents and are associated among others with impaired babe sleep [4], parental exhaustion [5] and depression [6], and shaken baby syndrome [seven]. For some families, the excessive crying is and so unbearable that infants are hospitalized to alleviate parental stress and family disturbance [viii]. Merely in 5% of excessively crying infants, however, a medical crusade for the crying can exist plant [ane]. Effects of behavioral methods to reduce infant cry and fuss problems in infants younger than six months have rarely been reported and do non take into account the immaturity of newborn babies [9]. New solutions that reduce baby crying are therefore warranted.
Such a possible solution is the Happiest Baby method (HB) [10]. According to Karp [x] recreating the sensory milieu of the womb (e.thou., snug position, floating in fetus position, deep resonant sound of the placental blood flow, jiggling motility, swallowing of amniotic fluid) would calm infants by triggering infants' then called calming response (CR). In HB infants' CR is triggered via a bundle of v stimuli or steps (5S's) the moment the baby is crying: (i) swaddling; (2) side/stomach position in the arms of the parent; (3) shushing; (four) swinging; and (five) sucking. Each of these 5S'southward has a calming effect on infants [11–16]. Infants who were simultaneously soothed with the 5S's by a researcher after immunization showed decreased duration of crying and lower mean pain scores compared to infants who were given either h2o or sucrose pre-vaccination and were soothed as usual postal service-vaccination [17]. HB also appears to significantly decrease infants' excessive crying in infants under 4 months of age [18].
HB, all the same, requires the availability of the parent to soothe the baby, only parents also demand rest, for example during the nighttime. Following Kurth'southward arrangement model on baby crying and maternal fatigue [5], infants who are easily soothed allow their parents to recover. Well-rested parents are ameliorate able to take care of their infants and to aid them regulate themselves, resulting in more successful soothing and positive parent-kid interactions. On the reverse, parents who take enduring difficulties with soothing their baby do non get enough remainder. Equally a consequence, information technology is more than difficult for exhausted parents to showroom adequate parenting behaviors and to at-home the baby, causing the infant to cry even more. Parent and infant may and then end up in a vicious circle, in which the baby and the parent bring each other out of balance time after time. A solution to break this barbarous circle or to prevent parental exhaustion may lie in mechanical soothing. Recently, a smart crib has been adult that makes use of three of HB'southward 5S'due south (swaddling, shushing via white noise, and swinging) to at-home infants [xix]. If mechanical soothing using swaddling, sound, and move is as effective as parental soothing using the same stimuli, mechanical soothing may be used during periods in which the wearied parent needs residuum, such equally during the night.
Infant'due south age might influence the strength of the CR. According to Karp [10], infants would actually need a quaternary trimester in the womb to mature, which is biologically incommunicable. In the beginning 3 months later birth, infants would notwithstanding be very sensitive to these intra-uterine stimulations, subsequently which the CR would gradually diminish. By then infants would exist able to regulate themselves better and are more fix for the requirements of the extra-uterine environment. This fits with evidence that infants experience the kickoff biobehavioral shift effectually 3 months, during which the behavior and physiology of infants shifts from intra-uterine to more extra-uterine regulation [twenty–23]. Around this period, several changes in infants' behavioral development can be observed [22]: a more diurnal sleep/wake bike appears with longer periods of consolidated sleep during the night, infants bear witness enhanced habituation and classical and operant conditioning, and begin to prove more responsive socially-oriented behavior, such as eye-contact, grinning, and cooing. On a more physiological level, infants are as well more than able to regulate themselves and become less dependent on their caregivers. For instance, Van Puyvelde et al. [23] constitute that infants adapted their respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) levels to their mothers' RSA levels in the first two months of life, but this relationship disappeared at three months of historic period. Thus, offering calming sensory stimuli for soothing might be more of import for younger than for older infants.
To date, no study has compared parental elicitation of the CR with a mechanical device that elicits such a response nor investigated whether infant age affects the strength of the CR. Obtaining more insight in offer sensory stimuli past unlike "providers" (i.due east., parent or device) for soothing would enable us not only to investigate whether human contact is needed to elicit a CR, just would too enable (infant) mental health professionals to fine-tune the guiding and treatment of both infants and parents for inconsolable crying. Nigh studies investigating the calming effects of sensory stimuli focused on a single stimulus, whereas nosotros assessed infants' CR to a combination of stimuli. We also adopted a multimethod arroyo past using behavioral observations and physiological measures to report the CR.
We examined infants' CR to parental and mechanical soothing using a combination of swaddling, audio, and movement. The post-obit questions were examined: (ane) Practice swaddling, audio, and movement by ways of parental and mechanical soothing induce a CR in infants?; (2) Is the CR stronger in younger than in older infants?; and (three) Is there a difference in the strength of the calming response betwixt parental and mechanical soothing? The force of the CR was examined using behavioral observations of infant fussiness and physiological measures of arousal, that is centre rate (Hour), which reflects the balance of the sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous system [24], and heart rate variability (HRV) in the high frequency domain, a relatively pure index of parasympathetic activity [25]. Nosotros hypothesized that a CR in response to swaddling, sound, and movement would be demonstrated past a significant decrease of observed infant fussiness, and a physiological pattern of relaxation, or in other words, a shift towards less sympathetic and more parasympathetic activation, equally reflected in a significant decrease of 60 minutes, and a meaning increase in HRV, equally compared to induced distress. In addition, we expected that the younger the historic period of the infant, the stronger the CR of the infant. Every bit in both soothing atmospheric condition swaddling, sound, and movement were used, no differences in the strength of the CR were expected betwixt parental and mechanical soothing.
Materials and methods
Participants
Participants were 69 infants (37 boys) and one of their parents (67 mothers). Gestational age of the infants was on average 39.58 weeks (SD = 1.78, range xxx.29−41.86 weeks). Three infants were born with a gestational age beneath 37 weeks. For these infants, corrected age was calculated using the chronologic age and adjusting for gestational age, that is, for the number of additional weeks from term (37 weeks). The (corrected) hateful age of the infants was xiii.85 weeks (SD = half-dozen.57, range = 3.99−27.19 weeks). Parents were on boilerplate 33.43 years (SD = iii.63, range = 26.40−42.50 years). Virtually all parents were married/cohabitating (n = 66) and born in the Netherlands (n = 62). Parents had a relatively high educational level (One thousand = 6.64, SD = .64, on a calibration from 1 (primary schoolhouse) to 7 (university)). Participants were recruited through leaflets provided by child care centers, midwives, general practitioners, and the obstetrics department of two hospitals in Amsterdam, and via websites and Facebook groups that parents visit oftentimes. Parents were asked to participate in a study on the calming effects of swaddling, motility, and audio on infants. Parents received information almost the study beforehand and had to sign informed consent. The study was approved past the ethical committee of the Research Institute of Child Development and Education of the University of Amsterdam (number 2017-CDE-7556). The individual depicted in Fig 1 has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent course) to publish these case details.
Overview of the experiment: Two atmospheric condition (smart crib/parent) with three phases (baseline, supine, soothing).
The individual in this figure has given written informed consent (as outlined in PLOS consent form) to publish these case details.
Procedure
Parents could sign themselves up for our study via our website. Parents that wanted to participate were contacted by phone to explicate the study and to make an appointment for a visit to our laboratory, and received a detailed information letter by electronic mail. Lab visits took place on different times during the day and were scheduled after a nap of the babe. At the lab visit, parents and infants were generally at ease. The experimenter first took the parent and babe to our waiting room, which looks like a cozy living room, and made sure the parent and infant were feeling comfortable before going to our lab room for the experiment. Parents were asked to make certain that their babe was non hungry or needing a diaper modify before starting the experiment. If the infant was hungry, parents get-go fed their kid before the experiment was started. The experimenter offset explained the procedure and parents signed an informed consent class. Then, the experimenter practiced the soothing (shushing and swinging) of the baby with the parent, to ensure that all parents soothed their infants in the same style. To prevent that the contact with the parent during the swaddling already calmed the infants, the babe was swaddled in a swaddle sack by the experimenter. After electrodes were fastened to the infant for the physiological measures, the experimental chore to measure infants' CR started, which lasted 12 minutes (for a description of the job, see Measurements). The parent and the infant were filmed during the job with 4 video cameras for behavioral measures that were coded afterwards. At the end of the experiment, parents received a refund of travel expenses and were taught HB [10] as a thank y'all gift. Parents' physiological data were as well recorded during the experiment and parents completed a set of questionnaires, but these data are not reported in the present report.
Measurements
Setting and procedure
Infants' CR was assessed with an experimental job that consisted of two weather condition (parent, smart crib) each equanimous of iii phases (baseline, supine, soothing) (see Fig ane). Each phase lasted ii minutes. During the baseline phase of the parent condition, the parent sat on a chair with the baby on his lap. The parent was allowed to quietly interact and instructed to move equally picayune as possible. Then, the supine condition started. The experimenter took the baby over from the parent, and of a sudden lowered her hands rapidly almost 20 centimeter and then abruptly stopped, a process that usually elicits the Moro reflex [26]. The experimenter and then immediately put the infant on his back on a mat on the floor two meters away from the parent. The parent was instructed to turn around and to refrain from whatever verbal and non-verbal contact with the infant. The baby and so lied on his back for two minutes, while the experimenter, out of sight of the baby, kept an heart on the kid. Both the eliciting of the Moro-reflex and/or the unavailability of the parent were expected to have a distressing outcome on the infant. Then, the experimenter swaddled the infant in a swaddle sack, while she did not interact with the baby (i.due east., making no eye contact and with a neutral facial expression). The parental soothing phase then started. The parent soothed the infant with the HB method [10]: the infant was handed over to the parent, and the parent held the child in a side position (back of the infant confronting the chest of the parent) and made a shushing sound close to the ear of the infant while swinging the infant (i.e., making pocket-size jiggly movements while supporting the head of the babe).
Subsequently the experimenter took the infant out of the swaddle sack and handed the infant to the parent, the baseline stage of the smart crib status started. Once again, the parent and the babe sat together on a chair for two minutes. And so the second supine stage started in which fussiness was once again elicited. Thereafter, the infant was once more swaddled in a swaddle sack. Lastly, during the smart crib soothing phase, the infant was placed supine in the crib and the swaddle sack was attached to the crib. The crib was put on the highest speed and sound level, moving horizontally half dozen cm either side during 3.2 cycles per 2d, and with white noise playing on 84dB. The parent was over again instructed to plow around and to make no contact with the babe. The experimenter over again made sure the baby was ok while out of sight of the infant. Order of the smart crib status and parent status was counterbalanced beyond infants.
Behavioral coding of infant fussiness
Our coding scheme was based on two well-known and validated coding schemes: the Laboratory Temperament Assessment Battery (Lab-TAB Prelocomotor version) [27], and the AFFEX arrangement for the coding of facial expressions [28].
Baby fussiness was observed in both supine and both soothing phases. Infant fussiness could not reliably be observed during both baseline phases, equally infants were mainly looking at their mother, out of sight of the camera. Babe fussiness was based on split up codings of: (1) duration of babe vocal expressions of fussiness (e.thousand., crying, whining); (2) intensity of vocal expressions of fussiness; (3); negative facial expressions (i.e., facial tension: potent mouth, squinted eyes, frowning); and (iv) motor activity. For the coding of all variables, each stage of two minutes was divided into 10 sec. fourth dimension intervals. Duration of infant vocal fussiness was coded in number of seconds per interval. All other variables were coded on a 4-point scale ranging from 0 to three. Higher scores indicated a higher frequency and/or intensity of that behavior. Final scores of each variable were obtained by standardizing and then averaging the scores across fourth dimension intervals. Cronbach'southward alpha of the four babe fussiness variables was .92, indicating a high level of internal consistency. A mean score of infant fussiness per stage was created by averaging the scores on the four variables.
Infant fussiness was coded by eight students, trained by the first author. 20 percent of videotapes were coded by all students to determine interobserver reliability. Mean interobserver reliability (intraclass correlations; ICC) was .97 (range .88–one.00).
Physiological measures
During the six chore phases, an ECG for parent and kid was taken with a Polar H7 Bluetooth middle charge per unit belt. The inter beat intervals (IBI's) betwixt R-peaks sent by the Polar were recorded with Vsrrp98 version 10.5 [29], a Windows information acquisition program developed by the technical staff of the Psychology department of the University of Amsterdam. Because the Polar center rate belt was too large for use with infants, nosotros modified the device and then that we could directly connect disposable ECG electrodes (3M Ruby Dot) to the H7 sensor. At the start of each measurement phase a marker was sent to the recording program Vsrrp98. Hr was calculated as the number of IBI's per minute. HRV was calculated as the root mean square of successive differences in IBI's (RMSSD), where intervals larger than 133% and smaller than 67% of the preceding IBI were rejected to remove artefacts from the signal. The RMSSD in IBI's reflects high-frequency variations indicative of parasympathetic activation [thirty,31]. Mean HR and HRV per minute was calculated per ii-infinitesimal phase.
Statistical analyses
Data were unremarkably distributed beyond all three phases per condition for each effect variable. Analyses were conducted separately for observed infant fussiness, HR, and HRV. We used 2-level (task phase inside children) multilevel regression models with restricted maximum likelihood estimation to account for dependency in the outcome variables.
Initially, multilevel models were conducted separately for the smart crib and parent status, and separately for observed infant fussiness, Hr, and HRV. This resulted in six multilevel models. In each multilevel model, we first compared the baseline to supine stage, to investigate whether the supine phase had the desired stressful effect on the infants (manipulation bank check). Secondly, to exam our primary hypothesis (i.eastward., whether infants responded with a CR to a combination of audio, movement, and swaddling), we compared the supine to the soothing phase. Thirdly, nosotros compared the soothing stage to the baseline phase, to investigate whether the infants were calm or calmer during the soothing than during rest, providing information nearly the extent of relaxation of the infant in the soothing phase. Of note, baseline phases were non included in the multilevel models of observed infant fussiness, as baby fussiness was not coded during the baseline phases. For observed infant fussiness, we thus simply straight compared the supine to soothing phase.
To examine whether infant age affected the force of the CR, we added infant age to these 6 multilevel models and investigated whether there was a significant interaction between infant age and the effect size for the difference in infant fussiness, Hour, and HRV between the supine to the soothing phase.
Lastly, to investigate whether the CR was stronger during the smart crib or parent condition, information of the two conditions were combined and the interaction between condition (smart crib, parent) and phase (supine, soothing) was tested in another multilevel model. Statistical significance was evaluated at α < .05. Data were analyzed with SPSS version 22 [32].
Results
Descriptive statistics and preliminary analyses
For two of the 69 participating infants, infant fussiness could not exist observed as audio recordings failed. The experiment was ended prematurely for one baby as the female parent indicated that her infant was likewise upset to continue. For one infant, Hour and HRV information were non bachelor for the smart crib condition, due to technical issues. Data on observed baby fussiness were thus available for 66 infants, and data on HR and HRV for 67 or 68 infants, depending on the status.
An immediate Moro response (abduction of the arms, adduction of the arms, and crying) was elicited in 40% of the infants. Infants in which the reflex was elicited were significantly younger (Thou = 10.65 weeks, SD = v.29) than infants in which the reflex was non elicited (Grand = 16.21 weeks, SD = 6.86), t(66) = iii.57, p = .001. Although the Moro was not elicited in all infants, they all displayed a certain level of behavioral fussiness during both supine phases (i.e., on the behavioral observations of elapsing of babe song expressions of fussiness, intensity of vocal expressions of fussiness, negative facial expressions, and motor activity), indicating that our manipulation succeeded.
Hateful levels of observed infant fussiness, infant HR, and babe HRV in the unlike phases and conditions are presented in Tabular array ane and Figs ii–iv, respectively.
Mean observed baby fussiness during the supine and soothing phases in the parent (N = 66) and smart crib condition (N = 66).
Error confined represent standard errors.
Hateful baby HRV during the baseline, supine, and soothing phases in the parent (N = 68) and smart crib condition (North = 67).
Error bars represent standard errors.
Table 1
Parent | Smart crib | |||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Baseline | Supine | Soothing | Baseline | Supine | Soothing | |||||||
Variables | N | Thou (SD) | Northward | M (SD) | N | M (SD) | North | M (SD) | N | One thousand (SD) | N | Yard (SD) |
Fussiness | - | - | 66 | .46 (.76) | 66 | -.60 (.77) | - | - | 66 | .48 (.77) | 66 | -.34 (.85) |
HR | 68 | 148.95 (xiii.48) | 68 | 151.37 (15.35) | 68 | 146.32 (sixteen.79) | 67 | 152.62 (fifteen.26) | 67 | 154.83 (19.47) | 67 | 143.84 (17.44) |
HRV | 68 | 16.25 (five.73) | 68 | 14.22 (five.96) | 68 | 15.88 (6.29) | 67 | 16.38 (6.forty) | 67 | xiv.02 (5.80) | 67 | 13.07 (v.17) |
Hateful infant Hour during the baseline, supine, and soothing phases in the parent (Northward = 68) and smart crib condition (N = 67).
Error bars represent standard errors.
Infants' calming response
Observed infant fussiness
Infant fussiness significantly decreased from supine to parental soothing, B = -ane.05, SE = .xi, t(65) = -9.44, p < .001, and also significantly decreased from supine to soothing by the crib, B = -.81, SE = .12, t(65) = -half dozen.96, p < .001.
In the parent condition, the supine-soothing × infant age interaction was significant, B = .04, SE = .01, t(64) = ii.threescore p = .012. The older the baby, the less strong the decrease in infant fussiness from supine to parental soothing. In the smart crib condition, the supine-soothing × infant historic period interaction was not meaning, B = -.00, SE = .02, t(64) = -.08, p = .933.
The subtract in infant fussiness from supine to soothing did not differ significantly betwixt weather condition, B = -.24, SE = .14, t(65) = -1.73 p = .088, although in that location was a trend for a stronger CR in response to soothing past the parent than past the smart crib.
Baby HR
During the parent condition, baby HR did not significantly differ between the baseline and supine phase, B = 2.42, SE = one.73, t(67) = i.40, p = .167. Infant Hour significantly decreased from supine to parental soothing (B = -5.05, SE = 2.14, t(67) = 2.35, p = .021). Infant Hr during parental soothing did non significantly differ from HR during baseline, B = -2.63, SE = two.27, t(67) = -1.16, p = .250.
During the smart crib condition, infant HR did not significantly differ between the baseline and supine stage, B = 2.20, SE = i.90, t(66) = 1.16, p = .251. Baby HR significantly decreased from supine to soothing by the crib (B = -10.98, SE = 2.26, t(66) = -4.85, p < .001), Moreover, infant HR during soothing by the crib was significantly lower than during baseline, B = -eight.78, SE = 2.32, t(66) = -3.79, p < .001.
In both the parent and smart crib condition, the supine-soothing × infant age interaction was not significant, B = .15, SE = .33, t(66) = .05, p = .964, and B = .17, SE = .34, t(65) = .48, p = .630 respectively.
Lastly, we investigated whether the CR was stronger during the parent or smart crib condition. The decrease in 60 minutes from supine to soothing was significantly stronger in the smart crib status than in the parent status, B = -5.96, SE = two.63, t(66.65) = -two.27, p = .027. Thus, infants responded with a stronger CR in 60 minutes to soothing by the smart crib than by the parent.
Infant HRV
During the parent condition, baby HRV significantly decreased from baseline to supine, B = -1.98, SE = .90, t(66) = -2.19, p = .032. Infant HRV did not significantly differ between the supine and soothing by the parent stage, B = 1.61, SE = .88, t(65.002) = 1.83, p = .073, but there was a trend for a higher HRV during parental soothing than during supine. Baby HRV did non differ betwixt the baseline and parental soothing phase, B = -.37, SE = .ninety, t(66) = -37, p < .683.
Regarding HRV during the smart crib condition, babe HRV significantly decreased from baseline to supine, B = -2.36, SE = .88, t(66) = -2.68, p = .009. Babe HRV did not significantly differ betwixt the supine and soothing past the crib phases, B = -.95, SE = .78, t(66) = one.21, p = .230. In improver, infant HRV was significantly lower during soothing by the crib than during baseline, B = -3.31, SE = .87, t(66) = -iii.78, p < .001.
In the parent condition, the supine-soothing × babe age interaction was significant, B = -.26, SE = .13, t(65.52) = -2.00, p = .049. The older the baby, the less strong the increment in infant HRV from supine to parental soothing. In the smart crib condition, the supine-soothing × infant age interaction was not significant, B = -.17, SE = .12, t(65) = -one.53, p = .131.
Lastly, we investigated whether the CR was stronger in the parent or smart crib status. Infants responded with a stronger calming response in HRV to soothing by the parent than past the smart crib, B = -two.69, SE = 1.07, t(62.98) = ii.53, p = .014.
Give-and-take
The purpose of this study was to examine (1) whether swaddling, sound, and movement by means of parental and mechanical soothing elicited a CR in infants; (2) whether infant historic period affected the forcefulness of the CR; and (3) whether there was a difference in the forcefulness of the CR between parental and mechanical soothing. Infants responded with a CR to a combination of swaddling, sound, and motility during parental and mechanical soothing in terms of infant fussiness and HR. For HRV, at that place was a trend for parental soothing to elicit a CR in infants, but mechanical soothing did not. Regarding the effects of infant age on the CR, it was found that younger infants responded with a stronger CR (decreased fussiness and increased HRV) to parental soothing, just not to mechanical soothing. Results regarding the differences in the strength of the CR betwixt parental and mechanical soothing were equivocal: for HR, infants' CR was stronger in the crib than in the parent condition, whereas for HRV, infants' CR was stronger in the parent condition. For fussiness, infants' CR tended to be stronger in the parent condition.
Our results advise occurrence of a CR in response to both soothing techniques. Soothing via swaddling, sound, and movement had a calming consequence on observed infant fussiness and on baby physiological activation: infants were observed to be less fussy and had a lower 60 minutes when soothed by the parent and the smart crib compared to lying supine. The consistency of the behavioral and HR outcomes suggests that a coordinated CR appears when soothing techniques are used to recover from the distressing situation of lying supine. Infants' HR during the soothing phases was fifty-fifty lower than during baseline (although not significantly for parental soothing), suggesting that infants' CR is fundamentally different, and appears more than relaxed, than infants' physiological country during placidity interaction.
Unexpectedly, parental and mechanical soothing did not clearly result in a shift to more parasympathetic activation, equally infants' HRV did not significantly differ betwixt the supine and soothing by the crib and parent phases. Our finding that at that place was a trend for a higher HRV during parental soothing than during supine was in accordance with expected relaxation during the CR. The slight, but statistically not-significant, subtract during mechanical soothing was opposite to the expected management. I may question whether lying supine was sufficiently lamentable to induce a shift towards more sympathetic and less parasympathetic arousal, assuasive u.s. to notice a subsequent CR on the HRV level during the soothing phases. Indeed, the increases in HR during the supine phases were statistically non-meaning as compared to baseline. HRV, however, significantly decreased during the supine phases compared to baseline, clearly reflecting the expected parasympathetic withdrawal associated with distress. So, the absence of meaning Hour increases during lying supine as compared to baseline may exist attributable to the slightly arousing nature of the baseline, rather than to the presumed mild stress of lying supine. That is, during the baseline phases, parents were instructed to move as niggling as possible and to interact only quietly with their baby. The absence of movement and sound might have been mildly stressful for infants. Alternatively, one may advise to interpret the absence of an increment in HRV during the mechanical soothing every bit a novelty response. This interpretation is, however, not supported by the observational and HR outcomes, as i would then besides expect an increase in HR and less observed behavioral relaxation. Moreover, such a novelty response would not last for two minutes. Taken the previous in consideration, we tend to interpret this HRV finding during mechanical soothing equally a spurious consequence, resulting in some inconsistency regarding the outcomes during mechanical soothing.
We expected that the younger the babe, the stronger the CR in response to swaddling, sound, and movement, every bit swaddling, audio, and motility imitate intra-uterine sensations [10], and equally infants' behavior and physiology gradually shifts from intra-uterine to more actress-uterine regulation during the first months [20–23]. We found some evidence for this hypothesis for parental soothing (for fussiness and HRV), but not for mechanical soothing. That younger infants were not more sensitive to the calming effects of swaddling, sound, and movement by the crib than older infants, might exist due to the standardized calming of the crib. The crib e'er used the same intensity of move and white dissonance, whereas swinging and shushing may have varied in terms of intensity during the parental soothing. As a result, even older infants seem to take reacted with an intense CR to soothing past the crib. Thus, our study showed that the CR response is likewise present in infants over three months of historic period. As adults are still sensitive to motion [33] and sound [34] to calm down and even autumn asleep, the CR in response to audio and motion may rather be an innate and universal characteristic of humans than a unique characteristic of newborns.
The results on the differences in the strength of the CR between parental and mechanical soothing were equivocal. For HRV, infants' CR was stronger in the parent status than in the crib condition. For infant fussiness, parental and mechanical soothing did not significantly differ in bringing nearly a CR in infants, although in that location was a tendency for infants to take a stronger CR in response to parental soothing. On the contrary, infants showed a stronger CR in terms of 60 minutes when soothed by the crib than past the parent. Thus, it remains unclear whether parental or mechanical soothing is more effective for calming infants. As humans are inherently social creatures, one might have expected that infants would exist more than hands soothed by their parent than past a mechanical instrument. Because of their immaturity at nascency, infants depend on their caregiving environment to survive and thrive. Indeed, maternal absence or unavailability is associated with infant physiological dysregulation and social withdrawal [35], showing that the mother has an important regulatory function [36]. Our study, nevertheless, shows that also mechanical soothing has a calming effect on infants, and may thus be a co-regulator of infants' beliefs and physiology. The crib might be an important help for parents. For instance, parents may quickly become wearied which poses a chance for parental mental health and sensitive and responsive interaction with the infant, which may consequence in a vicious cycle of increased infant crying and parental exhaustion [5]. During the night, aiming to prolong infant sleep and to decrease the number of night wakings, the smart crib would not supervene upon the parent, but would supersede a conventional crib. Parents would, nonetheless, respond to the needs of their baby similarly to when using a normal crib.
The finding that parental soothing using swaddling, shushing, and swinging had a direct calming effect on infants is in concordance with the written report of Harrington et al. [17] that showed that the 5S's of HB as applied by a researcher calmed infants later on immunization. Our report shows that HB can also exist easily taught to parents. Parents only shortly practiced with the experimenter how to shush and swing their infant. A recent report likewise showed that excessive crying in infants under 4 months can exist significantly diminished past the use of HB [xviii]. HB thus seems an intervention that tin can be straight used by parents and professionals to soothe infants.
In the present study, we only investigated the effects of parental and mechanical soothing using swaddling, sound, and motion on infants' CR, merely these sensory stimuli as well amend infant sleep. Swaddling has been constitute to make infants slumber longer [37], white dissonance promotes falling comatose quicker [38] and promotes waking less during the night [39], and movement accelerates slumber onset and induces deeper sleep [33,40]. The simultaneous use of the 5S's as well seem to boost baby sleep. Paul et al. [41,42] found that in a population of infants at adventure for obesity, predominantly breastfed infants receiving responsive care including HB slept more during the night than predominantly breastfed infants non receiving HB. In addition, daily sleep duration also increased for excessively crying infants who were soothed with HB and who were sleeping swaddled and with white noise on [18].
HB might, however, be too challenging if parents are exhausted and sleep deprived. The adjacent step would be therefore to investigate whether the smart crib can as well exist used to enhance baby sleep and thereby parental sleep. The smart crib might reduce the need of parental interference during the night, and equally a result parental night wakings and sleep elapsing might better. Especially reducing parental fragmented sleep seems important, since this has a detrimental effect on parental mood [43]. Past improving parental sleep via baby soothing and sleep, the vicious cycle between infant crying and sleeping problems on the 1 hand and parental burnout on the other hand may be broken. When rested, parents may be better able to meet the needs of the baby and to react in a more sensitive and responsive mode, which can contribute to a ameliorate bonding betwixt parent and infant [5]. Results from our first pilot report on the effectiveness of the crib showed that infant crying decreased and infant and maternal sleep improved during intervention with the crib compared to baseline [44].
The results of our study should be interpreted with several limitations in mind. Firstly, the results of our study must be interpreted carefully, equally we did non correct for multiple testing. Relatedly, some of the non-significant findings might be due to power issues, given the relatively low number of parents and infants in our report sample. Replication of our findings with larger samples is necessary.
Secondly, the level of elicited distress was but balmy. On the ane hand, this could have resulted in an underestimation of potential effects of our soothing methods on behavioral and physiological characteristics of distress. On the other hand, it remains unknown whether a CR can still be evoked using swaddling, audio, and movement when infants experience more than intense distress. Some support for the calming effects of swaddling, sound, and motion in response to a more intense stressor comes, withal, from the written report of Harrington et al. [17] that showed that the 5S's at-home infants afterwards immunization, which is a painful procedure for infants. Nevertheless, studies investigating the CR when inducing a college level of infant distress are needed.
Thirdly, baby fussiness could non be observed in the baseline phases, considering most infants were looking at their parents and cameras could not capture infants' faces. During the pilot testing of the study, nosotros tried to permit all infants sit still at their parents lap with their back against the breast of the parent so that infants' faces could be fully filmed, only this position made some infants fussy. Equally the baseline phase should represent a calm state, parents could ultimately choose themselves how they would similar to sit down with their babe, equally long every bit parent and child moved as little as possible.
Fourthly, we did not make up one's mind the effectiveness of the separate sensory stimuli as swaddling, sound, and motility were applied simultaneously. Although there is already evidence for a calming consequence of each of the 3 forms of stimulation, experimental studies might be useful for disentangling which or which combination of the 5S's is virtually constructive for eliciting the CR of infants. For some infants, swaddling may be enough, whereas some infants may exist more sensitive to movement, and other infants respond more to sound, or need a combination of (some of) the stimuli.
Fifthly, most participating parents were female and from a high social economic background, and near infants were born term, possibly limiting the generalizability of our findings. In improver, parents and infants were from a customs sample, and non specifically at high risk for excessive crying. Every bit the CR is assumed to be a universal infant response [ten], we expect a CR can be elicited in all infants using swaddling, sound, and motion, also in excessively crying infants. Results from our studies indeed show that swaddling, sound, and motility has a calming effect on excessively crying infants, when soothed by the parent with HB [18], and with the smart crib [44]. Future studies should compare the elicitation of the CR with parental versus mechanical soothing in excessively crying infants. Another interesting population to study is preterm born children, who oft have difficulty with sensory modulation (i.east., regulating the intensity of responses to sensory stimuli), which results in greater than typical irritability to sensory stimulation [45]. Preterm infants, withal, may exist especially responsive to the sensations that infants experienced in the womb considering of their immaturity. Information technology has been suggested that behavioral state regulation in preterm born infants may exist impaired due to lack of intra-uterine entrainment [46]. Preterm infants exposed to cycled light cried less and were less fussy at 5 and xi weeks corrected age than preterm infants exposed to dimmed light [46,47]. However, preterm infants' pattern of crying after their due date resembled that of term-built-in infants [48]. This may indicate that preterm infants likewise demand intra-uterine stimuli corresponding to the prolonged intra-uterine effect or the period before the first biobehavioral shift [22,23]. This might provide opportunities for early interventions directed at the babe's sensory processing as these may help to enhance the self-regulatory capacities of the preterm infant.
To conclude, parental and mechanical soothing using swaddling, sound, and movement promptly induced a CR in infants. This indicates that newborns are very sensitive to these intra-uterine stimuli. This finding might have important clinical implications for the soothing of fussy and crying infants and specifically in the context of parental exhaustion. Futurity studies are needed to investigate the effects of parental versus mechanical soothing in the home setting.
Supporting information
S1 File
Dataset infant crying and the calming response.
(SAV)
S1 Table
Codebook dataset.
(DOCX)
Acknowledgments
The authors are grateful for the participation of all families and would also like to thank Bert Molenkamp for his help with the processing of physiological data.
Funding Statement
The authors received no specific funding for this piece of work.
Data Availability
All relevant information are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.
References
1. Lucassen PLBJ. Systematic review of the occurrence of infantile colic in the community. Curvation Dis Child. 2001;84: 398–403. 10.1136/adc.84.v.398 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
two. Wessel MA, Cobb JC, Jackson EB, Harris GS, Detwiler AC. Paroxysmal fussing in infancy, sometimes called colic. Pediatrics. 1954;14: 421–435. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
3. LaHaye W, Engelberts AC, Tiemens IKF, Van Vlimmeren LA, De Ruiter M, Lucassen PLBJ, et al. Multidisciplinaire richtlijn "Preventie, signalering, diagnostiek en behandeling van excessief huilen bij babe's" [Multidisciplinary guideline "Prevention, detection, diagnostics, and treatment of excessive infant crying"]. Utrecht: Nederlands Centrum Jeugdgezondheid; 2013. [Google Scholar]
4. White BP, Gunnar MR, Larson MC, Donzella B, Barr RG. Behavioral and Physiological Responsivity, Sleep, and Patterns of Daily Cortisol Production in Infants with and without Colic. Child Dev. 2000;71: 862–877. 10.1111/1467-8624.00196 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
v. Kurth Eastward, Kennedy HP, Spichiger East, Hösli I, Zemp Stutz E. Crying babies, tired mothers: What exercise we know? A systematic review. Midwifery. 2011;27: 187–194. ten.1016/j.midw.2009.05.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
6. Wake M., Prevalence, Stability, and Outcomes of Weep-Fuss and Sleep Problems in the First 2 Years of Life: Prospective Community-Based Written report. Pediatrics. 2006;117: 836–842. 10.1542/peds.2005-0775 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
seven. Barr RG, Trent RB, Cantankerous J. Historic period-related incidence curve of hospitalized Shaken Babe Syndrome cases: Convergent show for crying as a trigger to shaking. Child Abuse Negl. 2006;30: seven–16. ten.1016/j.chiabu.2005.06.009 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
viii. Zwart P, Vellema-Goud MGA, Brand PLP. Characteristics of infants admitted to infirmary for persistent colic, and comparing with good for you infants. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96: 401–405. 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00090.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
9. Douglas PS, Loma PS. Behavioral Slumber Interventions in the First Six Months of Life Do non Improve Outcomes for Mothers or Infants: A Systematic Review. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2013;34: 497–507. 10.1097/DBP.0b013e31829cafa6 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
10. Karp H. The happiest baby on the block: The new way to at-home crying and assistance your newborn baby sleep longer. New York: Bantam Books; 2015. [Google Scholar]
eleven. Brackbill Y, Douthitt TC, Due west H. Psychophysiologic furnishings in the neonate of prone versus supine placement. J Pediatr. 1973;82: 82–84. 10.1016/S0022-3476(73)80017-4 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
12. Byrne JM, Horowitz FD. Rocking as a soothing intervention: The influence of direction and type of movement. Babe Behav Dev. 1981;4: 207–218. 10.1016/S0163-6383(81)80024-0 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
13. Erkut Z, Yildiz S. The Outcome of Swaddling on Hurting, Vital Signs, and Crying Duration during Heel Lance in Newborns. Pain Manag Nurs. 2017;18: 328–336. 10.1016/j.pmn.2017.05.007 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
xiv. Esposito K, Yoshida South, Ohnishi R, Tsuneoka Y, Rostagno M del C, Yokota S, et al. Infant Calming Responses during Maternal Carrying in Humans and Mice. Curr Biol. 2013;23: 739–745. 10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.041 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
15. Karakoç A, Türker F. Effects of White Noise and Holding on Pain Perception in Newborns. Pain Manag Nurs. 2014;15: 864–870. ten.1016/j.pmn.2014.01.002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
16. Tsao JCI, Evans S, Meldrum Thou, Altman T, Zeltzer LK. A Review of CAM for Procedural Hurting in Infancy: Part I. Sucrose and Non-Nutritive Sucking. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. 2008;5: 371–381. 10.1093/ecam/nem084 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
17. Harrington JW, Logan Southward, Harwell C, Gardner J, Swingle J, McGuire E, et al. Constructive Analgesia Using Physical Interventions for Infant Immunizations. Pediatrics. 2012;129: 815–822. 10.1542/peds.2011-1607 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
18. Möller EL, Oort FJ, Rodenburg R. Effects of a Responsive Soothing Intervention on Infant Excessive Crying and Infant Sleep. Manuscript under review. 2018. [Google Scholar]
19. Karp HN, Berlin MR, Gray JV, Washabaugh BW, Roy DK. Infant calming/sleep-help, sids prevention device, and method of use. Washington, DC; US20150045608A1. [Google Scholar]
twenty. Einspieler C, Marschik PB, Prechtl HFR. Human Motor Behavior: Prenatal Origin and Early Postnatal Evolution. Z Für Psychol/J Psychol. 2008;216: 147–153. 10.1027/0044-3409.216.3.147 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
21. Mirmiran M, Lunshof South. Perinatal development of human cyclic rhythms. Prog Brain Res. 1996;111: 217–226. 10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60410-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
22. Zeanah CH, Boris NW, Larrieu JA. Infant Evolution and Developmental Take a chance: A Review of the Past 10 Years. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36: 165–178. 10.1097/00004583-199702000-00007 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
23. Van Puyvelde One thousand, Loots K, Meys J, Neyt X, Mairesse O, Simcock D, et al. Whose clock makes yours tick? How maternal cardiorespiratory physiology influences newborns' centre charge per unit variability. Biol Psychol. 2015;108: 132–141. x.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.04.001 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
24. Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Emotion and motivation In: Cacioppo JT, Tassinary LG, Berntson GG, editors. Handbook of psychophysiology. 3rd ed New York: Cambridge University Press; 2007. pp. 581–607. [Google Scholar]
25. Penttila J, Helminen A, Jartti T, Kuusela T, Huikuri HV, Tulppo MP, et al. Time domain, geometrical and frequency domain analysis of cardiac vagal outflow: effects of diverse respiratory patterns. Clin Physiol. 2001;21: 365–376. 10.1046/j.1365-2281.2001.00337 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
26. Futagi Y, Toribe Y, Suzuki Y. The grasp reflex and moro reflex in infants: hierarchy of archaic reflex responses. Int J Pediat. 2012; Article ID 191562. ten.1155/2012/191562 [PMC gratis article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
27. Goldsmith HH, Rothbart MK. The laboratory temperament assessment battery: Prelocomotor version 3.one. Madison: University of Wisconsin-Madison; 1999. [Google Scholar]
28. Izard CE, Dougherty LM, Hembree. A system for identifying affect expressions past holistic judgments (AFFEX) Newark, DE: Calculator Network Services and University Media Services, Academy of Delaware; 1983. [Google Scholar]
29. Molenkamp B. VSRRP98 Transmission, Version x.v. Amsterdam: University of Amsterdam; 2017. [Google Scholar]
30. Malik G, Bigger JT, Camm AJ, Kleiger RE, Malliani A, Moss AJ, et al. Heart rate variability: Standards of measurement, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Eur Centre J. 1996;17: 354–381. 10.1093/oxfordjournals.eurheartj.a014868 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
31. Job Strength of the European Society of Cardiology and the North American Society of Pacing and Electrophysiology. Heart Rate Variability: Standards of Measurement, Physiological Interpretation, and Clinical Use. Circulation. 1996;93: 1043–1065. 10.1161/01.CIR.93.five.1043 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
32. SPSS statistics for Windows. Armonk, NY: IBM; 2013.
33. Bayer L, Constantinescu I, Perrig Southward, Vienne J, Vidal P-P, Mühlethaler M, et al. Rocking synchronizes brain waves during a brusque nap. Curr Biol. 2011;21: R461–R462. x.1016/j.cub.2011.05.012 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
34. Bellesi M, Riedner BA, Garcia-Molina GN, Cirelli C, Tononi Yard. Enhancement of slumber slow waves: underlying mechanisms and applied consequences. Front Syst Neurosci. 2014;viii: 209 10.3389/fnsys.2014.00208 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
35. Feldman R, Granat A, Pariente C, Kanety H, Kuint J, Gilboa-Schechtman E. Maternal depression and anxiety beyond the postpartum year and baby social engagement, fright regulation, and stress reactivity. J Am Acad Kid Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48: 919–927. x.1097/CHI.0b013e3181b21651 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
36. Feldman R. Parent-infant synchrony and the structure of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and run a risk weather condition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48: 329–354. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.ten [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
37. Van Sleuwen Exist, Engelberts Air-conditioning, Boere-Boonekamp MM, Kuis West, Schulpen TWJ, L'Hoir MP. Swaddling: A Systematic Review. Pediatrics. 2007;120: e1097–e1106. ten.1542/peds.2006-2083 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
38. Spencer JA, Moran DJ, Lee A, Talbert D. White noise and sleep induction. Arch Dis Kid. 1990;65: 135–137. x.1136/adc.65.1.135 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
39. Forquer LM, Johnson CM. Continuous White Noise to Reduce Resistance Going to Sleep and Night Wakings in Toddlers. Kid Fam Behav Ther. 2005;27: 1–10. 10.1300/J019v27n02_01 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
xl. Woodward S, Tauber ES, Spielman AJ, Thorpy MJ. Effects of Otolithic Vestibular Stimulation on Sleep. Sleep. 1990;13: 533–537. x.1093/sleep/xiii.6.533 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
41. Paul IM, Savage JS, Anzman SL, Beiler JS, Marini ME, Stokes JL, et al. Preventing Obesity during Infancy: A Pilot Study. Obesity. 2011;xix: 353–361. 10.1038/oby.2010.182 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
42. Paul IM, Savage JS, Anzman-Frasca S, Marini ME, Mindell JA, Birch LL. INSIGHT Responsive Parenting Intervention and Babe Slumber. Pediatrics. 2016;138: e20160762–e20160762. 10.1542/peds.2016-0762 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
43. Goyal D, Gay C, Lee Thou. Fragmented maternal slumber is more strongly correlated with depressive symptoms than infant temperament at three months postpartum. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2009;12: 229–237. 10.1007/s00737-009-0070-9 [PMC gratis article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
44. Van Meegen MME, Möller EL, Oort FJ, Rodenburg R. Potential effects of a smart crib on infant crying and sleep, maternal slumber and downheartedness. Manuscript nether review. 2018 [Google Scholar]
45. Bröring T, Oostrom KJ, Lafeber HN, Jansma EP, Oosterlaan J. Sensory modulation in preterm children: Theoretical perspective and systematic review. PLOS ONE. 2017;12: e0170828 10.1371/periodical.pone.0170828 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
46. Guyer C, Huber R, Fontijn J, Bucher HU, Nicolai H, Werner H et al. Cycled light exposure reduces fussing and crying in very preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2012;130: e145–e151. 10.1542/peds.2011-2671 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
47. Guyer C, Huber R, Fontijn J, Bucher HU, Nicolai H, Werner H, et al. Very preterm infants show earlier emergence of 24-hour sleep–wake rhythms compared to term infants. Early on Hum Dev. 2015;91: 37–42. 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2014.11.002 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
48. Bilgin A, Wolke D. Regulatory issues in very preterm and total-term infants over the outset 18 months. J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2016;37: 298–305. x.1097/DBP.0000000000000297 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
Which Method Is Not an Effective Way of Soothing a Crying Baby?
Source: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6481793/
0 Response to "Which Method Is Not an Effective Way of Soothing a Crying Baby?"
Post a Comment